- Chapter
Five -
Aboriginal
fringe dwellers in Darwin:
cultural persistence or culture of resistance? |
Reaching across difference: the Burarra people of central Arnhem Land
In this chapter I introduce my mostly Burarra-speaking interlocutors of Fish Camp and Lee Point who have moved from their homelands near the Blyth River in central Arnhem Land (see Map 2). I examine the past and present relationship between Aboriginal language groups of the Liverpool and Blyth River regions of Arnhem Land and the settlement at Maningrida, established by the Federal Government in 1957. I suggest that the Aboriginal homelands, or outstation movement, which began in Arnhem Land, parallels the resistance of the Burarra fringe camps in Darwin as a reaction against state control of Aboriginal lives. I give examples which suggest that resistance by the Burarra people in Darwin fringe camps is an attempt at engagement, or ‘reaching across difference’, which belongs to a tradition demonstrated since White settlement at Port Darwin. Finally, I construct a model of this process by adapting the Yolngu concept of ganma to an urban metaphor of merging traffic. The town of Maningrida, on the west bank
of the mouth of the Liverpool River, derives its name from a local place-name
meaning ‘the place where the Dreaming changes shape’ (Carew et al 1996a).
In per capita terms, Maningrida is perhaps the most multilingual community
in the world (Carew et al 1996b), with most people speaking or understanding
four or more of the fifteen languages from the region: Ndjebbana, Eastern
Kunwinjku, Kune, Rembarrnga, Dangbon/Dalabon, Nakkara, Gurrgoni, Djinang,
Wurlaki, Ganalpingu, Gupapuyngu, Kunbarlang, Gun-nartpa, Burarra and English
(Carew et al 1996b). Burarra is the common language of Maningrida (Glasgow
1985:7). The Burarra language group is also described by Benn (1994:iii)
as ‘a federation of Aboriginal people, concentrated on the Blyth River,
living mainly along the coast between Maningrida and Cape Stewart’. |
While referring to the language used
for most internal communications as ‘Burarra’, the people at Fish Camp
acknowledge the differences between An-barra speakers (a sub-community
of Gidjingali [Meehan 1982:12; Hamilton 1981:3; Corn 2001]), other close
dialects of the Blyth River region and the Gun-nartpa dialect of the Cadell
River region (see Glasgow 1985:7; Hamilton 1981:3; Green 1987:1).(1) According to Glasgow
(1985:7): ‘The Burarra and Gun-nartpa people ... number approximately
600, making up the predominant part of the Burarra Language Family’.(2)
Hiatt and Hiatt (1966:1) note that: ‘"Burara" is a term originally used
in eastern Arnhem Land for two groups who knew themselves as "Gidjingali"
and "Gunadba"’. Hamilton (1981:3) observed that by 1981 Burarra was the
inclusive name for all the groups of the Blyth River region and the term
Gidjingali was ‘never used’. This coincides with my observations in Darwin.
Many of the Fish Camp people claim Djunawunya
as their clan estate, four kilometres west of the Blyth River mouth, which
has been described in detail by Hiatt (1982; see also Meehan 1982:14),
celebrated in the popular song ‘Sunset Bay’ (Wild Water 1996)(3)
and shown in the film Waiting for
Harry (McKenzie 1980).(4)
Kopanga, on the coast, was the nearest outstation (see Meehan and Jones
1980) until the shift inland to Je-bena in about 1985, forty-four kilometres
from Maningrida, where water is reliable and the road to Maningrida is
open all year (Carew and Handelsmann 1996b). In 1958 the Djunawunya landowners identified
themselves as An-barra speakers (Hiatt 1982:21) who comprised almost half
of the total Gidjingali population of 600. However, Hiatt (p.21) recounts
how groups circulated for rituals and to exploit resources. Hiatt (p.15)
claims, ‘access and benefit [of resources] are normally accorded to a
wide network of tribesmen over and above the actual owners’ in the interests
of ‘an over-riding ethic of hospitality and open-handeness’. Hiatt’s (p.15)
observation, that ‘degrees of open-handeness bear a rough correspondence
to degrees of relateness’, differs from Sansom’s descriptions of ‘performative
kinship’ and a service economy, but corresponds with the ethic of sharing
in the fringe camps where the membership is drawn from related, though
wide-ranging groups. |
Carew et al (1996b) describe linkages
and overlapping between Burarra-speakers, the Yolngu dialects to the east,
‘the Nakkara’ (‘sometimes included with the Gijingarliya [Gidjingali]
group’) between Maningrida and the Blyth River, and the ‘Ndjebbana-speaking
Kunibidji’ [Gunavidji] people who are the traditional owners of the Maningrida
town area. These connections are often reflected in the mixed gatherings
at Fish Camp with residents comfortably switching in and out of the above
languages. In the fringe camps where I did my fieldwork
between 1996 and 2001, the members of the groups converse amongst themselves
mostly in their native languages, with English as a second, third or fourth
languag(5)
In contrast, Sansom (1980a:11) likens the fringe camp to ‘Babel’ where
people ‘ethnically unlike, will speak in different ways and so have different
words for things. They therefore cannot share properly in understandings’.
He then asks: ‘If ethnicity does not serve as a basis for association,
what else can?’ (Sansom 1980a:12). One response to the mixed nature of
the ‘Wallaby Cross’ mob is the adoption of ‘Aboriginal English’, or Kriol,
as the ‘prime camp language’ (Sansom 1980a:29). However, unlike ‘Wallaby
Cross’, where ‘the distinctiveness of rough camp English is that its very
roughness makes it English that is unwhite’ (Sansom 1980a:31),(6) Kriol was
rarely used at Fish Camp and Lee Point and there was never a demand that
‘Aboriginal English’ be used as a common language. Almost all of the Aboriginal people who
were associated with the Fish Camp community at some time during my fieldwork
originate from the central coastal region of Arnhem Land, which was declared
an Aboriginal Reserve in 1948 (see Map 2). In comparison, the people of
‘Wallaby Cross’ came from a hinterland of fragmented Aboriginal Reserves,
alienated land and cattle stations, which has a long history of contact
(see Sutton and Palmer 1980:17; Sansom 1980a:iii). In Arnhem Land, Aboriginal
land ownership is comparatively secure and social organisation is relatively
intact. My interlocutors always explain their relationship to each other
in kinship terms, such as ‘I call Dulcie grannie’, or use more specific
Burarra titles such as mununa (mother’s mother - see Hiatt 1965:48),
galikali (spouse) as well as
the sixteen subsection names recorded by Hiatt (1965:49) and Glasgow (1985:925). The majority of the regular Aboriginal
users of the camps at Fish Camp and Lee Point are members of the Gidjingali
clans of the Blyth River region discussed by Hiatt (1965, 1982, 1986b),
Meehan (1982:16), Hamilton (1981), Bagshaw (1982:50, 1994) and Meehan
and Jones (1986). The Gidjingali people, who generally refer to themselves
as An-barra or Burarra, are also predominant in camps at Palmerston and
around the Darwin suburb of Tiwi. I suspect that the movement of people
from the Blyth River region to Darwin has been partly because their homelands
are located between the Aboriginal towns of Maningrida and Milingimbi.
With no regular direct road service, access to services and goods is mostly
through the traditional lands of rival clans. Bob Bunduwabi came from Yilan, to the
east of Blyth River, and had close ties to the Yolgnu people from northeastern
Arnhem Land. His niece, Dulcie, had a Nakara father and An-barra mother
with close family extending into the Maung, and as far west as the Gundjeihmi
dialect groups. Her partner was a Djinang speaker from the Ramingining
area and a renowned singerman who often returned for funerals and other
ceremonies in the region. His family connections extended to Barunga,
south of Katherine, where Kriol is more widely used. When groups from
this region came to visit, the tensions at Fish Camp were noticeably increased.
Although some men from the extended family group had lived with women
from Central Australia, these languages are not as well understood and
visitors with affinal connections through these relationships seldom stayed
long in the camp. Fish Camp therefore accommodated a group who were closely
related, spoke closely related dialects and came from adjoining estates,
predominantly the areas described by Hiatt (1965,1982), Meehan (1982)
and Meehan and Jones (1980). The two main exceptions were the husbands
of two women in the core group. Apart from the Djinang man, there was
a 71-year-old man married to an elderly Burarra woman who had been a patient
at the leprosarium. The man was of mixed descent but identified as a Larrakia.
He looked after his wife by shopping and fishing with his cast net. He
drowned while on a night fishing excursion near the camp in 1998 (see
NT News, November 21). While these men
were accepted, tension was created during one week when a single urban
Aboriginal woman evicted from her home sought refuge in the camp. With
no kinship ties, and unaware of the expected behaviour towards kin, this
woman offended a male relative of the camp doyen and had to seek my protection.
She moved out soon afterwards. |
The Fish Camp group is typical of the
mixed ‘residential aggregates’ model that Sutton (1999a:26) derives
from his thorough analysis of Hiatt’s (1965:24) descriptions of Gidjingali
social groups, called ‘communities’ in Hiatt’s text.(8)
In another a Northern Territory Aboriginal town camp, Doohan (1992:79)
noted: ‘I have not experienced a situation where Aboriginal people without
some immediate kin ties at Aputula would take up residence there’. Similarly,
those who camped at Fish Camp from August 1996 to January 1998 for periods
of between two weeks and six months, with numbers peaking at twenty-five
between September and October 1997, were almost all from central Arnhem
Land. My experience confirms the observation
by Heppell and Wigley (1981:64): The town camp provides a recognised order
and ready community to which visitors can attach themselves. They can
be sure of obtaining shelter among kin with whom they have an established
set of reciprocal obligations, and can be reasonably certain that everyone
else living in the camp, if not immediate kin, are members of the same
tribal group and therefore, linked by historical ties of amity. My record of residence over time (Figure
3) indicates a shifting population of more than 150 ‘countrymen’ (and
women) who associated with Fish Camp for varied lengths of time. However,
in a submission for housing assistance, Simmering (1999; see Appendix
II) states that Fish Camp had a core group of twelve who identified
twenty-seven others who the group wished to accommodate on visits ‘of
weeks, months or longer’ (see also Appendix III).(9)
Ages ranged from three to older than eighty, although children were
rarely present. As Doohan (1992:75) found at an Aboriginal camp near
Finke, it is possible for an individual to identify with more than one
location. In Chapter Three I gave some first impressions
of the camp. Although I lived in the camp from May 1997 to January 1998
and visited regularly in other months, the language barrier I describe
above precludes a comprehensive ethnographic analysis of life in a fringe
camp. In addition, I maintain that the ‘classifying practices’ of the
hegemonic power noted by Asad (1993:17), Abu-Lughod (1990:47), Kapferer
(1995:88) and other analysts of resistance are a reasonable cause for
‘ethnographic refusal’ in studies of resistance amongst subaltern groups
like the fringe dwellers.(10)
As I have cited, authorities know little about the campers. Scott (1990:xi,
1985:321) notes that ‘sequestered settings’, ‘offstage’ and ‘removed
from institutional circuits’ are sites where resistance may be fostered.
I suggest that anonymity remains the fringe dwellers' strongest defence.
However, for the benefit of the reader unfamiliar with Darwin Aboriginal
fringe camps, the following section gives descriptions of everyday life
in one such 'sequestered setting' during my fieldwork before the camp
was closed in 1999. |
5.2 Some observations of life at
Fish Camp: 1996-8 During the dry season, the shelters at
Fish Camp were used only for storage. People shifted their ‘sit-down’
camp into the shade of the surrounding trees as the sun warmed, and
continued dragging tarpaulins and mattresses around with the shade
as it shifted throughout the day. The tents were used as a windbreak
during the colder nights when heavier logs were gathered in the evening
for the night fires. Sleepers laid their bedding close to, and around
the two or three hearths, depending on the numbers and family groupings
in the camp. If there was no alcohol to be consumed and no visitors
to entertain everyone settled down when darkness set in. Apart from
the half dozen dogs, who barked fiercely if anything or anyone approached,
there was usually little apparent concern for physical barriers or
other protection for the exposed sleepers. When there was alcohol being shared,
the group would stay up by the fires until very late at night, singing
and dancing to the clap-sticks and ngorla
(didgeridoo) kept at the camp, or sharing stories in a relaxed
mixed-gender circle sitting on the earth (I discuss drinking at Fish
Camp in Chapter Eight).(11)
At times of less abundance, the camp was often kept awake by domestic
arguments, fuelled by alcohol, which were highly repetitive night
after night. These arguments were usually over failure to reciprocate,
accusations of sexual unfaithfulness, allegations of talk behind the
back of the accuser or disputes over who was the most important representative
of the camp. These internal tensions are fuelled by alcohol and controlled
within boundaries of kinship and do not motivate the forms of resistance
described in this thesis.(12)
In a ‘miler’ week or on a Sunday when people have no cash or the convenient
liquor outlets are closed, silence reigns and people rest, go fishing
or gathering bush foods and recuperate.(13) Temperatures during the dry season nights
fall to as low as 16 degrees Celsius. Foam mattresses were dragged
closer to the fires for warmth, and it was not uncommon for bedding
to smoulder or for sleepers to suffer bad burns. Couples and single
women slept around their own fire apart from the single men and women.
Occasionally, if too many single men were drinking and the spouses
were not present, the single women, mostly middle-aged, brought their
blankets to my hearth for protection or removed themselves altogether
for the night. Early in the morning the fires were stoked to boil
tea and cook whatever food was available for a light breakfast. If
there was a major drinking session continuing, people would be woken
at first light to continue the celebrating and to share what alcohol
had been saved as a ‘reviver’ until the liquor outlets opened for
business. Sometimes a man, but more usually women,
gathered firewood in the late afternoon for themselves and the pensioners.
The fittest, younger men were often slow to help. At Fish Camp, the
firewood was mostly dry mangrove timber that was plentiful in the
tidal regions of the Kulaluk lease. In the wet season, there was no
attempt to keep wood dry, but fires could be started even on all but
the wettest days. In the dry season, mangrove wood burns relatively
quickly, leaving a fine ash that built up after a few nights until
it was eventually shovelled to the side of the cleared sleeping and
activity area. When a larger log of black wattle or similar better-quality
wood was burnt, it was allowed to smoulder continuously until it was
consumed. Occasionally a vehicle might help bring wood from more distant
areas for the fires that are a distinctive feature of the fringe camps.
Pieces of arc mesh or similar metal scraps were used as grills for
cooking, or pots were perilously balanced on the burning logs (see
Plate 4). Sparks flying, these burning branches were sometimes wielded
as weapons, more in spectacular threat at night than in blows, although
the scattered coals did create havoc with clothing and bedding. With no vehicle, and several disabled
pensioners, Fish Camp was reliant on taxis and ‘minibuses’ for trips
to the bank and shops. Fares are negotiable on the minibuses, which
can take up to thirteen people at a reasonable charge, making this
form of transport indispensable to fringe dwellers, particularly for
ceremonial gatherings and for protests. Unfortunately, at thirty dollars
for the round journey to the shops, the fares took a sizeable proportion
of the pensioner income on pension day. There were also friends who
came to take pensioners shopping, or to the bank to collect debts.
None at the camp had a drivers licence, so I sometimes drove a hired
a vehicle which was paid for by contributions from the camp. In later
months, the White activist friends began helping with shopping and
excursions. Fish Camp was rich in utensils, cooking
pots and fishing gear compared to other camps and even to many Aboriginal
homes. However, this was not always immediately evident because cups,
spoons, saucepans and plates lay scattered about on the ground or
hidden until they were needed. Occasionally there would be a clean
up and washed utensils would be stacked together but mostly they were
retrieved and washed when needed. Old cardboard made useful plates
for meat or shellfish or as a firelighter or fan. Empty plastic bottles
of all shapes and sizes were used to share out the wine that arrived
in four litre casks called ‘suitcases’ or ‘yellow boxes’. Once used,
the ‘plastic’, as the wine containers were called, was cast aside
without the top and not used again. There was little attempt to put
aside the bottles for the next drinking session or to wash and keep
them for later use. At Fish Camp the used drinking bottles were raked
into the piles of litter and put in bins that were removed weekly
by the Keep Australia Beautiful utility, one of the few services provided
to the camp. When money is short, groups went crabbing,
fishing with lines in the nearby creek or gathering food in the mangrove
swamps, including worms from dead trees, shellfish called ‘longbums’
(telescopium telescopium)
from the mud flats, periwinkles (nerita
lineata) clinging to mangrove trees and small oysters from the
rocks (see Plate 10). Wild honey, yams and berries were also collected
from the surrounding monsoon forest. The camp usually had at least
one cast-net that was used to catch smaller fish in shallow water.
Friends and family might bring a wallaby, geese in season, long neck
turtle or seafood from a hunting trip out of town. An urban Aboriginal
entrepreneur usually drove into camp on pay weeks with saltwater turtle
meat, offering the campers the cheaper, less saleable parts that made
a popular soup. However, most ‘bush tucker’ came from shops that specialised
in this type of food, including kangaroo tails, fish, shellfish and
live crabs. It was also quite common for groups from the camp to spend
a day out of town hunting or fishing when transport was available. |
5. I do not speak any Aboriginal languages. |
6. As I have described, Sansom (1980-82:6) elsewhere
draws a distinction between ‘that pidgin’ for ‘organizin for business’
and traditional language, or ‘lingo’, reserved for ‘High Culture’. |
11. Chairs were also popular when they were available.
When I purchased four steel-legged plastic chairs, they were constantly
being ‘borrowed’ from my area (see Plate 15). |
Contents
Appendices
Bibliography
Plates
Maps
Illustrations
Figures